Intended for healthcare professionals

News

Is the cognitive function of older patients affected by antihypertensive treatment? Results from 54 months of the Medical Research Council's treatment trial of hypertension in older adults

BMJ 1996; 312 doi: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.312.7034.801 (Published 30 March 1996) Cite this as: BMJ 1996;312:801
  1. Martin J Prince, lecturer in psychiatrya,
  2. Anne S Bird, consultant psychiatristb,
  3. Robert A Blizard, lecturer in statisticsc,
  4. Anthony H Mann, professor of epidemiological psychiatrya
  1. a Section of Epidemiology and General Practice, Institute of Psychiatry, London SE5 8AF
  2. b Royal Free Hospital, London NW3 2QG
  3. c Academic Department of Psychiatry, Royal Free Hospital, London NW3 2QG
  1. Correspondence to: Dr Prince.
  • Accepted 5 December 1995

Abstract

Objective: To establish whether initiation of treatment with diuretic or β blocker is associated over 54 months with change in cognitive function.

Design: A cognitive substudy, nested within a randomised, placebo controlled, single blind trial.

Setting: 226 general practices from the Medical Research Council's general practice research framework.

Subjects: A subset of 2584 subjects sequentially recruited from among the 4396 participants aged 65-74 in the trial of treatment of hypertension in older adults. The 4396 subjects were randomised to receive diuretic, β blocker, or placebo. Subjects had mean systolic pressures of 160-209 mm Hg and mean diastolic pressures <115 mm Hg during an eight week run in.

Outcome measures: The rate of change in paired associate learning test (PALT) and trail making test part A (TMT) scores (administered at entry and at 1, 9, 21, and 54 months) over time.

Results: There was no difference in the mean learning test coefficients (rate of change of score over time) between the three treatments: diuretic -0.31 (95% confidence interval -0.23 to -0.39), β blocker -0.33 (-0.25 to -0.41), placebo -0.30, (-0.24 to -0.36). There was also no difference in the mean trail making coefficients (rate of change in time taken to complete over time) between the three groups: diuretic -2.73 (95% confidence interval -3.57 to -1.88), β blocker -2.08 (-3.29 to -0.87), placebo -3.01, (-3.69 to -2.32). A less conservative protocol analysis confirmed this negative finding.

Conclusion: Treating moderate hypertension in older people is unlikely to influence, for better or for worse, subsequent cognitive function.

Key messages

  • Key messages

  • Studies have shown that treating hypertension in older adults reduces cardiovascular mortality and morbidity

  • Treating moderate hypertension with either diuretic or β blocker does not seem to influence cognitive function

  • Concerns about damaging cognition should not deter doctors from treating hypertension in older patients

  • Age should no longer be a factor in the decision to initiate antihypertensive treatment

Introduction

Until recently antihypertensive treatment was commonly withheld from older people because of two widely held opinions—namely, that high blood pressure was a healthy adaption to arteriosclerotic rigidity and that lowering blood pressure was fraught with complications. In 1989 a popular textbook of family medicine1 talked of high blood pressure in older people having “a different significance from high blood pressure in the young … A high blood pressure in the aged might actually have survival value.” It went on to warn that hypotensive drugs were “more likely to produce postural hypotension and confusion in the aged.” The first of these views is no longer tenable. In the past 15 years four randomised placebo controlled trials have established that treating moderate hypertension in older adults reduces cardiovascular and cerebrovascular morbidity and mortality.2 3 4 5 The second view, that antihypertensive treatment may be associated with unacceptable complications, in particular cognitive impairment, may deter physicians from initiating treatment in older subjects. This reluctance may have important adverse consequences on public health; the largest reduction in cardiovascular mortality in the 1980s has been among those aged 75 years and over.6

The Framingham study reported a modest inverse association between mean blood pressure over five years and cognitive function measured 12 to 15 years later.7 8 It was most evident among subjects who had not been treated with antihypertensive medication.7 Of course the existence of a relation between untreated hypertension and cognitive impairment need not imply that lowering blood pressure with drugs will confer benefit. Indeed, studies have suggested that too large a reduction in blood pressure may be associated with increased vascular morbidity.9 Farmer et al cited two studies that reported on patients with hypertension who were treated rather than untreated; two that reported poorer performance; and five that found no difference.8 10 In these studies the cognitive outcome may have been affected by factors influencing the decision to treat, such as the age of the subject, the degree of hypertension, and severity of vascular pathology. For this reason the balance of cognitive risks and benefits for older hypertensive subjects on antihypertensive treatment is best studied in a randomised controlled trial.

The Medical Research Council trial of treatment of hypertension in older adults compared the effects of a diuretic drug and a β blocker with placebo in a randomised, placebo controlled, single blind design.5 In a cognitive substudy we investigated the effects of the initiation of antihypertensive treatment and of lowering blood pressure on the cognitive performance of trial participants. We wanted to establish whether there is any medium term benefit or disadvantage for cognitive function associated with antihypertensive treatment for older people with moderate hypertension.

The Medical Research Council trial of the treatment of hypertension in older people5

Subjects

Screened volunteers: 4396

Period: 1982-9

Inclusion criteria:

  • Systolic pressure 160-209 mm Hg and mean diastolic pressure <115 mm Hg during eight week run in

  • Age 65-74 years Exclusion criteria:

  • Taking antihypertensive medication

  • Cardiac failure, angina, diabetes, asthma or other serious disease

  • Myocardial infarction or cardiovascular accident in previous 3 months

    The cognitive substudy to the Medical Research Council trial5

    Subjects

    The first 2680 recruits for the MRC trial; 29 refused, 2651 agreed

    Recruitment: 1983-5

    Inclusion criteria:

    Completion of main outcome cognitive test measures (PALT and TMT) at entry or 1 month, and at least one further follow up

    Exclusion criteria: None

    Final numbers qualifying:

  • PALT analysis 2567

  • TMT analysis 2584

    Method

    Setting: 226 general practices from the MRC research framework

    Design: Randomised, placebo controlled, single blind trial

    Randomisation:

  • Placebo (50%)

  • Atenolol 50 mg daily (25%)

  • Hydrochlorothiazide 25 mg plus amiloride

    2.5 mg daily (25%) (all medication adjusted and supplemented to keep blood pressure within target range)

    Baseline measures:

  • Systolic and diastolic blood pressure at entry, and annually for 5 years

  • Smoking habit on entry to the MRC trial (ex-smokers not distinguished from non-smokers)

    Method

    Design: A comparison of change in cognitive performance over time, nested within a randomised, controlled trial

    Measures of cognitive function (at entry, 1, 9, 21, and 54 months):

  • Paired associate learning test (PALT)14

  • Trail making test A (TMT)16

    Intelligence measures (entry only):

  • Raven's progressive matrices parts A and B (RM)21

  • New adult reading test (NART)20

    Other measures: Self-care-D22—a validated brief, self completed depression rating scale

    All administered by an appropriately trained MRC research nurse attached to each practice

    Outcome

    End points:

  • Strokes

  • Coronary events

  • All cardiovascular events

  • Deaths from all causes

    Mean follow up: 5.8 years

    Results: Significant reduction in strokes, coronary events, and all cardiovascular events in diuretic but not β blocker group

    Outcome

    Outcome measures:

  • Rate of change in PALT over time (PALT coefficient)

  • Rate of change in time taken to complete

    TMT over time (TMT coefficient)

    Follow up: 54 months

    Results: Preliminary analysis of the first nine months of the study suggested no effect on cognitive performance11

Method

Details of the trial of treatment of hypertension in older adults5 and a report of the first nine months of the substudy11 have already been published. Details of the subjects and methods relevant to this analysis are summarised in the box.

MEASURES OF COGNITIVE FUNCTION USED IN THE SUBSTUDY

A package of tests was constructed to test aspects of brain function at entry to the trial and at 1, 9, 21, and 54 months thereafter. Memory and attention are reported to be the cognitive domains most affected by raised blood pressure.7 12 13 Two measures were chosen to detect any change. Firstly, we used the paired associate learning test (PALT).14 This tests the capacity to recall the second half of a pair of words cued by the first half of the couplet therefore examining one component of cognitive function—semantic memory. Memory testing, however, is a core component of many general tests of cognitive function, and the learning test correlates well with brief tests of broader cognitive function such as the abbreviated mental test score and the psychogeriatric assessment schedule.15

Secondly, we used the trail making test (TMT) part A16 to test attention, concentration, and psychomotor function. Subjects are timed while they join consecutive numbers, arranged at random. There is evidence from magnetic resonance scanning studies that this test should be sensitive to early change in cognitive function related to hypertensive arteriopathy.17 18 19

At entry to the trial only, we used two measures of intelligence: the new adult reading test (NART)20—a stable measure of premorbid intelligence—and Raven's matrices parts A and B21—tests of non-verbal intelligence sensitive to age related cognitive decline. The scores from the learning test and the trail making test at entry and at 1, 9, 21, and 54 months were converted into summary measures for each subject to reflect changes in their scores over time. The summary measures (PALT coefficient and TMT coefficient) were calculated as the slope of the regression lines derived by regressing the cognitive test scores on time for each subject. In the regression calculation time values of 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 were assigned respectively to the cognitive test scores at entry and 1, 9, 21, and 54 months as doing so led to a better linear fit for the regressions than using the actual time in months (see results).

ANALYSIS

We carried out two analyses. An intention to treat analysis dealt with subjects only according to their initial trial randomisation; subsequent changes in medication were ignored. A less conservative protocol analysis further subdivided subjects into nine treatment protocols according to whether they remained on their randomised allocation throughout the 54 months of the study or needed treatment with another antihypertensive agent to lower their blood pressure into the target range or had stopped their randomised medication (whether or not they required additional medication).

To assess the adequacy of the randomisation for the subset of trial participants used in the cognitive substudy the means of the initial trial characteristics were compared for those randomised to placebo, diuretic, and β blocker by using one way analysis of variance. Next, the mean learning test and trail making test coefficients for the three treatment groups were compared by using analysis of variance with adjustment for any non-randomly distributed characteristics. As the benefits of randomisation were lost in the protocol analysis the means and coefficients for each of the nine treatment protocols were compared by using analysis of variance with age, sex, trail making or learning test score (as appropriate) at entry, reading score (NART), Raven's matrices score, and entry self care D depression score entered as covariates, all of which were independently associated with both the learning and trail making coefficients. To illustrate the treatment effect on blood pressure the mean fall in systolic blood pressure (the difference between the systolic blood pressure at entry and the mean of subsequent measures) was compared across the three randomised groups and the nine protocols.

Results

Table 1 summarises the general characteristics of the sample by initial randomisation.

Table 1

Means (95% confidence intervals) and proportions (%) of baseline characteristics by randomisation in hypertension treatment trial

View this table:

Loss to follow up—For the trail making test 551 subjects (21% of the sample) recorded the maximum five data points, 1582 (61%) four or more, and 2263 (88%) recorded at least three. The figures for the paired associate learning test, administered during the same session, were very similar. During the course of the 54 month cognitive substudy 192 subjects (8%) died. Of these only 77 (3%) died during the first 21 months. If we grouped the subjects into three by mortality—death before 21 months, death between 21 and 54 months, and survival to the end of the study—we found no relation between either the cognitive test scores recorded at entry, the summary learning or trail making coefficients, systolic blood pressure at entry, or age and mortality during the trial. Smokers were less likely to survive to the end of the trial: 30% of those who died during the trial smoked compared with 20% of those surviving to 54 months (χ2 12.5; P=0.0004). By stratifying the sample by the number of data points recorded and excluding subjects who died during the course of the study we found a relation between loss to follow up for reasons other than death and several baseline measures (table 2). Those who completed the study were younger and less depressed at entry to the trial with less impaired entry scores on the learning test, trail making, and Raven's matrices. There was no relation between the score on the reading test and loss to follow up.

Table 2

Characteristics of sample (means and 95% confidence intervals) by number of recorded data points. Subjects dying during course of trial are excluded

View this table:

Changes in learning and trail making scores over time—In the estimation of the learning and trail making coefficients the time points were taken as ordinal values 0, 1, 2, 3, and 4 as this provided a better approximation to a straight line than using exact time points. Mean (SD) learning test scores deteriorated from 17.0 (1.7) at entry to the trial to 16.3 (2.2) at 54 months (table 3). The mean learning test coefficient was -0.30 (95% confidence interval -0.27 to -0.35; range -8.0 to 9.0). Lower scores indicated greater impairment, hence the more negative the coefficient the greater the deterioration in test performance over time. The distribution had a pronounced central tendency with most subjects recording similar learning test scores at each data point. The mean of the times taken to complete the trail making test actually improved steadily from 60.5 seconds at entry to 51.9 seconds at 54 months. The mean trail making coefficient was - 2.71 (-1.3 to -3.8; -145 to 195). This distribution also had a strong central tendency. Shorter times to complete the test indicated better performance; hence the less negative the coefficient the less the improvement in performance over time.

Table 3

Mean fall in systolic blood pressures* (in mm Hg) trail making and learning test coefficients by initial randomisation and course of subsequent treatment

View this table:

Intention to treat analysis—The first row of table 3 shows that there was no difference in the mean learning test or trail making coefficients between the three randomised allocations. Inclusion as a covariate of entry systolic blood pressure (the only baseline characteristic non-randomly distributed between the three treatment arms) did not modify this result.

Protocol analysis—Table 3 also shows the breakdown of the sample by initial randomisation and subsequent course of treatment, together with the effect of initial randomisation and subsequent treatment on the mean fall in systolic blood pressure over the 54 months of the trial. The fall in blood pressure was greater for subjects randomised to active treatment groups than for those randomised to placebo. This treatment effect was largest among subjects who completed the study on their original randomisation. Table 3 also gives the mean learning test and trail making coefficients for each of the nine possible treatment protocols. These have been adjusted for the independent predictors of the two coefficients. Despite the treatment effect on blood pressure, there was no evidence of significant heterogeneity in the cognitive outcome between treatment groups.

Discussion

This study prospectively assessed cognitive status in a large sample of older adults randomised to diuretic, β blocker, or placebo treatment groups. This was an ideal opportunity to investigate the relation between treatment of moderately raised blood pressure and cognition. The finding of no association between hypertensive treatment and cognitive outcome over 54 months is reassuring. The benefits of treatment with thiazide diuretic have been shown by the Medical Research Council's trial of the treatment of hypertension in older adults.5 Fears regarding adverse effects on cognition are not borne out in this analysis and need not influence clinicians in their decision to initiate antihypertensive treatment in an older patient.

There was a substantial attrition from the trial cohort. Little of this could be attributed to death. Subjects who failed to complete the study were older than those who completed it and showed impairment of current cognitive function (as measured by the paired associate learning test, the trail making test, and Raven's matrices) but not of premorbid function as measured by the new adult reading test at entry to the study. This would support the view that subclinical cognitive decline on recruitment was a factor in determining later drop out. Non-completion was also related to randomisation (table 2) in that subjects randomised to atenolol were more likely to be off medication for at least part of the study. It is doubtful, however, that loss to follow up will have influenced our results unduly. Inclusion of subjects with only two learning test or trail making values has meant that an outcome measure was available for 96% of trial subjects. Summary outcome measures based on fewer data points will be measured with more error, but this error is likely to be random. Inclusion of subjects with incomplete follow up therefore seemed to be the lesser of two evils. In any event, restriction of the analysis to those subjects who had completed the trial did not alter the result.

This analysis does not provide any evidence for a cognitive benefit from antihypertensive treatment. Even restriction of the analysis to those subjects who had completed the study and remained on their randomised medication throughout (thereby maximising the treatment effect on mean systolic blood pressure) did not reveal any difference in cognitive outcome between the placebo and active treatment groups. This result does not, however, prove that treating hypertension cannot benefit cognitive function in older adults. Though the learning test and trail making test should be sensitive to relatively small degrees of cognitive deterioration and should cover the cognitive domains most likely to be affected by hypertension more detailed cognitive testing may have revealed a treatment effect. The 54 month follow up period may have been too short to detect a difference between treated and untreated groups. In the Framingham cohort historical7 rather than concurrent10 blood pressure records were associated with cognitive impairment. Alternatively the subjects in this trial, who were 65 years or older at entry, may have been exposed to raised blood pressure for too long to benefit from this intervention.

We thank the MRC Working Party and the MRC Epidemiology and Medical Care Unit (director—Professor T Meade, study coordinator—Dr J Connelly, statistician—P Brennan), the MRC general practice framework general practitioners and research nurses (coordinator Mrs W Browne), and Mrs I Godstone.

Footnotes

  • Funding Dr Prince holds a clinical training fellowship in epidemiology funded by the Wellcome Trust.

  • Conflict of interest None.

References

  1. 1.
  2. 2.
  3. 3.
  4. 4.
  5. 5.
  6. 6.
  7. 7.
  8. 8.
  9. 9.
  10. 10.
  11. 11.
  12. 12.
  13. 13.
  14. 14.
  15. 15.
  16. 16.
  17. 17.
  18. 18.
  19. 19.
  20. 20.
  21. 21.
  22. 22.