Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current issue
    • Past issues
    • Early releases
    • Collections
    • Sections
    • Blog
    • Infographics & illustrations
    • Podcasts
    • COVID-19 Articles
    • Obituary notices
  • Authors & Reviewers
    • Overview for authors
    • Submission guidelines
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Forms
    • Editorial process
    • Editorial policies
    • Peer review process
    • Publication fees
    • Reprint requests
    • Open access
    • Patient engagement
  • Members & Subscribers
    • Benefits for CMA Members
    • CPD Credits for Members
    • Subscribe to CMAJ Print
    • Subscription Prices
    • Obituary notices
  • Alerts
    • Email alerts
    • RSS
  • JAMC
    • À propos
    • Numéro en cours
    • Archives
    • Sections
    • Abonnement
    • Alertes
    • Trousse média 2023
    • Avis de décès
  • CMAJ JOURNALS
    • CMAJ Open
    • CJS
    • JAMC
    • JPN

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
CMAJ
  • CMAJ JOURNALS
    • CMAJ Open
    • CJS
    • JAMC
    • JPN
CMAJ

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current issue
    • Past issues
    • Early releases
    • Collections
    • Sections
    • Blog
    • Infographics & illustrations
    • Podcasts
    • COVID-19 Articles
    • Obituary notices
  • Authors & Reviewers
    • Overview for authors
    • Submission guidelines
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Forms
    • Editorial process
    • Editorial policies
    • Peer review process
    • Publication fees
    • Reprint requests
    • Open access
    • Patient engagement
  • Members & Subscribers
    • Benefits for CMA Members
    • CPD Credits for Members
    • Subscribe to CMAJ Print
    • Subscription Prices
    • Obituary notices
  • Alerts
    • Email alerts
    • RSS
  • JAMC
    • À propos
    • Numéro en cours
    • Archives
    • Sections
    • Abonnement
    • Alertes
    • Trousse média 2023
    • Avis de décès
  • Visit CMAJ on Facebook
  • Follow CMAJ on Twitter
  • Follow CMAJ on Pinterest
  • Follow CMAJ on Youtube
  • Follow CMAJ on Instagram
Auscultations

Incidence of and risk factors for nodding off at scientific sessions

Kenneth Rockwood, David B. Hogan, Christopher J. Patterson and ; for The Nodding at Presentations (NAP) Investigators
CMAJ December 07, 2004 171 (12) 1443-1445; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1503/cmaj.1041570
Kenneth Rockwood
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David B. Hogan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christopher J. Patterson
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Tables
  • Responses
  • Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

We conducted a surreptitious, prospective, cohort study to explore how often physicians nod off during scientific meetings and to examine risk factors for nodding off. After counting the number of heads falling forward during 2 days of lectures, we calculated the incidence density curves for nodding-off episodes per lecture (NOELs) and assessed risk factors using logistic regression analysis. In this article we report our eye-opening results and suggest ways speakers can try to avoid losing their audience.

Despite their known inefficiency, lectures (“a means of transferring notes from the pages of the speaker to the pages of the audience, without going through the mind of either”) continue to predominate as a means of helping physicians learn their trade. At a recent 2-day lecture series, we noticed that many of the attendees around us were nodding off, including one of our coauthors (C.J.P.). After awakening him, we decided to study the boredom itself by measuring how often physicians nodded off during the lectures and assessing risk factors for this behaviour.

Figure
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint

Figure. Photo by: Earthlore

Methods

Since we were sitting together at the back of the room, we counted the number of heads falling forward as a sign of nodding off. We chose this method because counting is scientific. We carefully recorded data on what we thought seemed like reasonable risk factors; anything we were unsure of we made up. In as much as a single episode of nodding off indicates submaximal attention, we calculated incidence density curves. To be fair to the speakers (after all, we are Canadians), we counted only 1 nodding-off episode per listener-colleague per lecture. For the logistic regression analysis we dichotomized nodding-off events as occurring at a frequency above the median or, at or below, the median or less. Because this was an exploratory study, we also administered a short questionnaire (Appendix 1) to colleagues who had nodded off.

Results

About 120 people attended the 2-day lecture series. We had to adjust our analysis because many had left by the end of the second day. The quality of the lectures varied from entertaining and informative, to monotonous and repetitive, to rushed, to Felliniesque. The incidence density curve ranged from 3 nod-off episodes per lecture (NOELs) to 24 NOELs per 100 attendees (median 16 NOELs per 100) (Fig. 1). Risk factors for NOELs are presented in Table 1.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint

Table 1.

Figure
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint

Fig. 1: Special incidence density curve, showing number of nodding-off events per lecture (NOELs) per 100 attendees over length of time of presentation.

Interviews with colleagues who nodded off revealed that they were comforted to know they were not alone. Most had no enthusiasm to attend boring lectures but were inclined to go if influenced by payment, CME credits, guilt or obsessiveness. Being internists, all but 1 were relieved to discover that their falling asleep was not their fault but that of the speakers.

Interpretation

We observed that clinically important proportions of physicians nodded off during the lectures, that there appeared to be a dose–response effect and that speaker characteristics were the strongest risk factors.

Our study had important limitations. Because we sat at the back of the room, we could not see everyone's faces. Thus, people who can sleep without head movement would have been missed. However, since we were counting physicians who were “nodding off” and not “sleeping,” we were pretty much covered there. Misclassification bias was another possibility, especially since the rapid flashing of slides could have induced absence seizures that may have been mistaken for nodding-off events. Another limitation was one of undercounting, especially during lectures by the more boring speakers. Such speakers can induce inattention (and its common correlate, fantasy) to the extent that it becomes impossible to concentrate on the task of counting nodding heads. However, as far as we can tell, at least 2 of us were attentive at any given time, so we doubt that undercounting was a factor. Perhaps this is fantasy, though. Overcounting may have occurred if some of the NOELs were actually vigorous noddings in agreement (NIAs). However, experienced observers such as ourselves can readily distinguish between NOELs and NIAs by a variety of associated factors, including timing, amplitude, frequency, and presence of snoring, drooling and gasping. Narcolepsy, however, must remain in the differential diagnosis of NOELs.

Our study was not precisely double-blinded, since we could not find a valid way of unobtrusively counting people with our eyes closed. The frequent nodding off of one of us (C.J.P.) is a form of blindness, and, as is often the case, our colleagues had no idea of what we were up to. Therefore, we claim a one-and-a-half-blinded design. (This study design has received scandalously little formal attention from methodologists, something that one of us [C.J.P.], being located at McMaster University, hopes to put right, if he can stay awake.)

We were interested to observe that some intrinsically boring talks (those with obscure topics, few data, absent analyses) had unexpectedly low NOEL rates. We attributed this to the bizarreness of the presentation. Factors such as wandering off to inspect the screen, dropping the microphone or just raving — although disconcerting to the audience — helped to keep the physicians awake, as did side bets among attendees on when the speaker's prefatory comments would end and the actual topic of the lecture addressed.

We were surprised to see the relation between tweed and NOELs. Further analysis shows that it is tweed, not plaid, that is implicated. Tweed is often worn by fops, but many otherwise admirable men wear tweed from time to time without apparent adverse effects. Chronic tweed wearing, however, might indicate a boring phenotype, or it might be causal: tweed may harbour little insect-like creatures whose dander could cause asthma and chronic hypoxemia, with subsequent cerebral dysfunction. Without appropriate clinicopathological correlation it is impossible to say. Thus, we have resolved, in the interests of science, to sacrifice a few boring speakers and study their brains, pending ethical approval.

The questionnaire administered to the nodders-off was revealing. Most were reassured to know that it wasn't their fault. One participant, however, insisted on accepting the blame, and indeed on making sure that all physicians who nodded off were to be blamed entirely. We have encouraged this person to switch to a career better suited to physician-blaming, such as law, evidence-based medicine or bioethics. (The last option appears to be the most efficient for career change, often requiring no more than a mini-sabbatical and a willingness to preface even the most banal comments with “as Plato has taught us.”)

Nodding off at presentations is common and may pose a risk to the health of patients. Studies are required to assess the effectiveness of interventions (e.g., lessons in public speaking, wardrobe makeovers, drama classes) in preventing nodding off during lectures.

Appendix 1

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint

Appendix 1.

PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

Canadian Medical Association Journal: 171 (12)
CMAJ
Vol. 171, Issue 12
7 Dec 2004
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author

Article tools

Respond to this article
Print
Download PDF
Article Alerts
To sign up for email alerts or to access your current email alerts, enter your email address below:
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on CMAJ.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Incidence of and risk factors for nodding off at scientific sessions
(Your Name) has sent you a message from CMAJ
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the CMAJ web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Incidence of and risk factors for nodding off at scientific sessions
Kenneth Rockwood, David B. Hogan, Christopher J. Patterson
CMAJ Dec 2004, 171 (12) 1443-1445; DOI: 10.1503/cmaj.1041570

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
‍ Request Permissions
Share
Incidence of and risk factors for nodding off at scientific sessions
Kenneth Rockwood, David B. Hogan, Christopher J. Patterson
CMAJ Dec 2004, 171 (12) 1443-1445; DOI: 10.1503/cmaj.1041570
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Methods
    • Results
    • Interpretation
    • Appendix 1
  • Figures & Tables
  • Responses
  • Metrics
  • PDF

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • A cellular-telephone model of assessing frontal lobe function in physicians
  • Nodding and napping in medical lectures: an instructive systematic review
  • The study of NOELs
  • The study of NOELs
  • The study of NOELs
  • The study of NOELs
  • Yada Yada Yada or carpe somnum
  • Google Scholar

More in this TOC Section

  • Oh Christmas Tree, Oh Christmas Tree ...
  • A missed diagnosis
Show more Auscultations

Similar Articles

Collections

  • Topics
    • Medical careers

 

View Latest Classified Ads

Content

  • Current issue
  • Past issues
  • Collections
  • Sections
  • Blog
  • Podcasts
  • Alerts
  • RSS
  • Early releases

Information for

  • Advertisers
  • Authors
  • Reviewers
  • CMA Members
  • CPD credits
  • Media
  • Reprint requests
  • Subscribers

About

  • General Information
  • Journal staff
  • Editorial Board
  • Advisory Panels
  • Governance Council
  • Journal Oversight
  • Careers
  • Contact
  • Copyright and Permissions
  • Accessibiity
  • CMA Civility Standards
CMAJ Group

Copyright 2023, CMA Impact Inc. or its licensors. All rights reserved. ISSN 1488-2329 (e) 0820-3946 (p)

All editorial matter in CMAJ represents the opinions of the authors and not necessarily those of the Canadian Medical Association or its subsidiaries.

To receive any of these resources in an accessible format, please contact us at CMAJ Group, 500-1410 Blair Towers Place, Ottawa ON, K1J 9B9; p: 1-888-855-2555; e: cmajgroup@cmaj.ca

Powered by HighWire